More stories

  • in

    Novel wearable armband helps users of prosthetic hands to ‘get a grip’

    Typing on a keyboard, pressing buttons on a remote control or braiding a child’s hair has remained elusive for prosthetic hand users. With current myoelectric prosthetic hands, users can only control one grasp function at a time even though modern artificial hands are mechanically capable of individual control of all five digits.
    A first-of-its-kind study using haptic/touch sensation feedback, electromyogram (EMG) control and an innovative wearable soft robotic armband could just be a game changer for users of prosthetic hands who have long awaited advances in dexterity. Findings from the study could catalyze a paradigm shift in the way current and future artificial hands are controlled by limb-absent people.
    Researchers from Florida Atlantic University’s College of Engineering and Computer Science in collaboration with FAU’s Charles E. Schmidt College of Science investigated whether people could precisely control the grip forces applied to two different objects grasped simultaneously with a dexterous artificial hand.
    For the study, they also explored the role that visual feedback played in this complex multitasking model by systematically blocking visual and haptic feedback in the experimental design. In addition, they studied the potential for time saving in a simultaneous object transportation experiment compared to a one-at-a-time approach. To accomplish these tasks, they designed a novel multichannel wearable soft robotic armband to convey artificial sensations of touch to the robotic hand users.
    Results, published in Scientific Reports, showed that multiple channels of haptic feedback enabled subjects to successfully grasp and transport two objects simultaneously with the dexterous artificial hand without breaking or dropping them, even when their vision of both objects was obstructed.
    In addition, the simultaneous control approach improved the time required to transport and deliver both objects compared to a one-at-a-time approach commonly used in prior studies. Of note for clinical translation, researchers did not find significant differences between the limb-absent subject and the other subjects for the key performance metrics in the tasks. Importantly, subjects qualitatively rated haptic feedback as considerably more important than visual feedback even when vision was available, because there was often little to no visually perceptible warning before grasped objects were broken or dropped. More

  • in

    Smartphone app can vibrate a single drop of blood to determine how well it clots

    Blood clots form naturally as a way to stop bleeding when someone is injured. But blood clots in patients with medical issues, such as mechanical heart valves or other heart conditions, can lead to a stroke or heart attack. That’s why millions of Americans take blood-thinning medications, such as warfarin, that make it harder for their blood to clot.
    Warfarin isn’t perfect, however, and requires patients to be tested frequently to make sure their blood is in the correct range — blood that clots too easily could still lead to a stroke or a heart attack while blood that doesn’t clot can lead to extended bleeding after an injury. To be tested, patients either have to go to a clinic laboratory or use a costly at-home testing system.
    Researchers at the University of Washington have developed a new blood-clotting test that uses only a single drop of blood and a smartphone vibration motor and camera. The system includes a plastic attachment that holds a tiny cup beneath the phone’s camera.
    A person adds a drop of blood to the cup, which contains a small copper particle and a chemical that starts the blood-clotting process. Then the phone’s vibration motor shakes the cup while the camera monitors the movement of the particle, which slows down and then stops moving as the clot forms. The researchers showed that this method falls within the accuracy range of the standard instruments of the field.
    The team published these findings Feb. 11 in Nature Communications.
    “Back in the day, doctors used to manually rock tubes of blood back and forth to monitor how long it took a clot to form. This, however, requires a lot of blood, making it infeasible to use in home settings,” said senior author Shyam Gollakota, UW professor in the Paul G. Allen School of Computer Science & Engineering. “The creative leap we make here is that we’re showing that by using the vibration motor on a smartphone, our algorithms can do the same thing, except with a single drop of blood. And we get accuracy similar to the best commercially available techniques.”
    Doctors can rank blood-clotting ability using two numbers: the time it takes for the clot to form, what’s known as the “prothrombin time” or PT a ratio calculated from the PT that allows doctors to more easily compare results between different tests or laboratories, called the “international normalized ratio” or INR More

  • in

    Who’s watching? Nearly a third of TV ads play to empty rooms

    Paying thousands of dollars to advertise on television is a huge proposition — never more so than for the Super Bowl, for which 30-second TV spots this year will cost advertisers as much as $6.5 million. Contrary to Super Bowl advertisements, which are some of the most high-profile commercials, new Cornell University research shows nearly a third of TV ads play to empty rooms.
    Advertising pricing relies on measuring how many TVs are tuned in to a specific channel, and not whether people are actually watching the TVs.
    “We wanted to quantify whether the current industry standard is doing a good job predicting what advertisers care about,” said lead author Jura Liaukonyte, associate professor in the Cornell SC Johnson College of Business.
    For this research, the co-authors worked with TVision Insights, a TV performance metrics company that developed innovative technology to passively monitor who’s in the room and whether they’re actually looking at what’s on the TV screen, while respecting viewer privacy.The research analyzed 4 million ad exposures over the course of a year.
    Their findings — including the fact that nearly a third of TV ads play to empty rooms, and that viewers are four times more likely to leave the room than change the channel — are detailed in “How Viewer Tuning, Presence and Attention Respond to Ad Content and Predict Brand Search Lift,” which published Feb. 9 in Marketing Science.
    Among other results, the team found that ad viewing behaviors vary depending on channel, time of day, program genre, age and gender. For example, older viewers are more likely to avoid ads by changing channels; younger viewers are more likely to avoid ads by leaving the room or diverting their visual attention — likely due to multitasking with a second screen.
    Additionally, ads for recreational products — beer and video games, for example — do the best at retaining viewers, the researchers said. Among the worst at keeping eyes on the screen are prescription drug ads, particularly those for serious conditions.
    The Super Bowl, of course, is a different animal from every other show in the TV ad realm, the researchers said.
    “It has become sort of like the Oscars for the advertising industry,” Liaukonyte said.
    Cornell Chronicle version of story: https://news.cornell.edu/stories/2022/02/nearly-third-tv-ads-play-empty-rooms
    Story Source:
    Materials provided by Cornell University. Original written by Tom Fleischman, courtesy of the Cornell Chronicle. Note: Content may be edited for style and length. More

  • in

    A diamondlike structure gives some starfish skeletons their strength

    Some starfish made of a brittle material fortify themselves with architectural antics.

    Beneath a starfish’s skin lies a skeleton made of pebbly growths, called ossicles, which mostly consist of the mineral calcite. Calcite is usually fragile, and even more so when it is porous. But the hole-riddled ossicles of the knobby starfish (Protoreaster nodosus) are strengthened through an unexpected internal arrangement, researchers report in the Feb. 11 Science.

    “When we first saw the structure, we were really amazed,” says Ling Li, a materials scientist at Virginia Tech in Blacksburg. It looks like it’s been 3-D printed, he says.

    Li and colleagues used an electron microscope to zoom in on ossicles from several dozen dead knobby starfish. At a scale of 50 micrometers, about half the width of a human hair, the seemingly featureless body of each ossicle gives way to a meshlike pattern that mirrors how carbon atoms are arranged in a diamond.

    Zooming in on the bumpy growths called ossicles (seen in this electron microscope image) that make up a knobby starfish’s skeleton reveals a meshlike structure similar to the arrangement of carbon atoms in diamond. This arrangement strengthens the ossicles, which are mostly made of calcite, a relatively weak mineral.Ling Li/Virginia Tech

    But the diamondlike lattice alone doesn’t fully explain how the ossicles stay strong.

    Within that lattice, the atoms that make up the calcite have their own pattern, which resembles a series of stacked hexagons. That pattern affects the strength of the calcite too. In general, a mineral’s strength isn’t uniform in all directions. So pushing on calcite in some directions is more likely to break it than force from other directions. In the ossicles, the atomic pattern and the diamondlike lattice align in a way that compensates for calcite’s intrinsic weakness.

    It’s a mystery how the animals make the diamondlike lattice. Li’s team is studying live knobby starfish, surveying the chemistry of how ossicles form. Understanding how the starfish build their ossicles may provide insights for creating stronger porous materials, including some ceramics.

    We can learn a lot from a creature like a starfish that we may think is primitive, Li says.

    Sign Up For the Latest from Science News

    Headlines and summaries of the latest Science News articles, delivered to your inbox

    Thank you for signing up!

    There was a problem signing you up. More

  • in

    Where mathematics and a social perspective meet data

    Community structure, including relationships between and within groups, is foundational to our understanding of the world around us. New research by mathematics and statistics professor Kenneth Berenhaut, along with former postdoctoral fellow Katherine Moore and graduate student Ryan Melvin, sheds new light on some fundamental statistical questions.
    “When we encounter complex data in areas such as public health, economics or elsewhere, it can be valuable to address questions regarding the presence of discernable groups, and the inherent “cohesion” or glue that holds these groups together. In considering such concepts, socially, the terms “communities,” “networks” and “relationships” may come to mind,” said Berenhaut.
    The research leverages abstracted social ideas of conflict, alignment, prominence and support, to tap into the mathematical interplay between distance and cohesiveness — the sort evident when, say, comparing urban and rural settings. This enables adaptations to varied local perspectives.
    “For example, we considered psychological survey-based data reflecting differences and similarities in cultural values between regions around the world — in the U.S., China, India and the EU,” Berenhaut said. “We observed distinct cultural groups, with rich internal network structure, despite the analytical challenges caused by the fact that some cohesive groups (such as India and the EU) are far more culturally diverse than others. Mark Twain once referred to India as ‘the country of a hundred nations and a hundred tongues, of a thousand religions and two million gods.’ Regions (such as the Southeast and California in the U.S.) can be perceived as locally distinct, despite their relative similarity in a global context. It is these sorts of characteristics that we are attempting to detect and understand.”
    The paper, “A social perspective on perceived distances reveals deep community structure,” published by PNAS (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States) can be found here.
    “I am excited by the manner in which a social perspective, along with a probabilistic approach, can illuminate aspects of communities inherent in data from a variety of fields,” said Berenhaut. “The concept of data communities proposed in the paper is derived from and aligns with a shared human social perspective. The work crosses areas with connections to ideas in sociology, psychology, mathematics, physics, statistics and elsewhere.”
    Leveraging our experiences and perspectives can lead to valuable mathematical and statistical insights.
    Story Source:
    Materials provided by Wake Forest University. Original written by Kim McGrath. Note: Content may be edited for style and length. More

  • in

    Capturing hidden data for asymptomatic COVID-19 cases provides a better pandemic picture

    Asymptomatic COVID-19 cases are the bane of computer modelers’ existences — they throw off the modeling data to an unknown degree. You can’t measure what you can’t detect, right? A new approach from Los Alamos National Laboratory’s Theoretical Division, however, explores using historic epidemic data from eight different countries to estimate the transmission rate and fraction of under-reported cases.
    “Asymptomatic cases are the ‘dark matter’ of epidemics,” said Nick Hengartner, one of the authors on the report published today in the journal PLOS ONE. “We see only the indirect evidence that more people are sick than reported, and if we don’t account for them, we may wrongly conclude that the epidemic is under control. So we changed the model to focus on the observed counts instead of trying to model the ‘perfect’ world. By looking back through the time series of historical data, we can see from their dynamics what’s missing.”
    The importance of capturing the undocumented cases is significant, especially in a disease such as COVID-19, where asymptomatic individuals have accounted for 20-70 percent of all infections.
    Co-author Imelda Trejo, a postdoctoral fellow at Los Alamos noted, “This is a new extension of the standard SIR (susceptible-infected-recovered) epidemiological models to study the underreported incidence of infectious disease. The new model reveals that trying to fit an SIR model type directly to raw incidence data will underestimate the true infectious rate. This could actually lead decisionmakers to declare the epidemic under control prematurely.” Instead, the team presented a Bayesian method (a statistical model using probability to represent all uncertainty within the model) to estimate the transmission rate and fraction of underreported cases.
    As tested against the data of eight countries (Argentina, Brazil, Chile, Colombia, Mexico, Panama, Peru and the U.S.), the new approach directly describes the dynamics of the observed, under-reported cases. “We use the local dynamics of the observed cases to propose a model that gives us a conditional expectation of new cases, based on the observed history,” Trejo said.
    Story Source:
    Materials provided by DOE/Los Alamos National Laboratory. Note: Content may be edited for style and length. More

  • in

    Computational modelling experts pioneer pest-busting model

    Mathematicians at the University of Leicester have developed a new mathematical model which could greatly increase the efficiency of pest control and hence significantly reduce the impact of pests on crops whilst minimising the damage to environment.
    A new study, published in Scientific Reports, builds upon individual-based model (IBM) techniques to explain and predict the formation of high slug density patches in arable fields.
    While existing models built around the Turing theory of pattern formation (named for AI pioneer Alan Turing) and its generalisations are shown to work well for patterns in plant distribution, these are rarely able to accurately predict the distribution of animals due to the complexity of behavioural responses.
    Drawing on field data collected in a three-year project, computational modelling experts in the University of Leicester’s School of Computing and Mathematical Sciences, alongside colleagues from The University of Birmingham and Harper Adams University, applied mathematical concepts to build a new model which shows trends of distribution, accounting for the movements of individual creatures.
    Their model could be used in creating more efficient methods of pest control — by targeting the use of pesticides and other techniques to protect crops — and could be adapted to better understand the collective behaviour in other species, such as fish schools, bird flocks, and insect swarms.
    Sergei Petrovskii is a Professor in Applied Mathematics at the University of Leicester and lead author for the study. Professor Petrovskii said:
    “This study is an example of how a fundamental ecological concept, when applied to a real-world problem, can lead to breakthrough findings and ultimately helps to make agriculture more sustainable”
    Keith Walters, Professor in Agriculture and Pest Control at Harper Adams University, said:
    “Understanding factors determining slug distribution in agricultural fields have been a long-standing problem. Using unique field techniques specifically developed to support modelling and simulations allowed progress that would hardly be possible with empirical tools alone.”
    Dr Natalia Petrovskaya, Senior Lecturer in Applied Mathematics at the University of Birmingham and corresponding author for the study, added:
    “Computer simulations helped us to reveal a hidden link between biological processes going on very different spatial scales, which was crucial for the success of this project.”
    ‘A predictive model and a field study on heterogeneous slug distribution in arable fields arising from density dependent movement’ is published in Scientific Reports.
    Story Source:
    Materials provided by University of Leicester. Note: Content may be edited for style and length. More

  • in

    Spatial training with blocks and puzzles could unlock the UK's mathematical potential

    Spatial training with blocks and puzzles could unlock mathematical potential. A sustained focus on spatial reasoning training could help children learn science, technology, engineering and mathematics, according to new research from the University of Surrey.
    The Surrey study found that teaching spatial skills — particularly with the use of blocks, puzzles and other physical manipulatives — is highly effective at improving mathematics performance. The team also found that physical spatial reasoning training was far more effective than digital sessions.
    Dr Katie Lee-Gilligan, co-author of the study and Lecturer in Developmental Psychology at the University of Surrey, said:
    “Our research confirms that when children learn the relationship between space and shapes through tangible physical tools such as puzzles, their mathematics performance improves. This is critical information for us all, particularly parents, teachers and decision-makers, at a time when the UK is lagging behind its international competitors when it comes to STEM skills.”
    Spatial reasoning is defined as a person’s ability to think about shapes and space in two and three dimensions, and previous research has shown that spatial reasoning is crucial for daily living, for example, navigating to work, filling the dishwasher, and putting on your shoes.
    The research, which was conducted in partnership with the University of Toronto and the University of Maryland, also highlights the importance of not restricting the teaching of spatial reasoning to young children as they found evidence of mathematical gains in older groups past the age of seven.
    Dr Zack Hawes, co-author of the study and Assistant Professor at the University of Toronto, commented:
    “Despite these and other findings that demonstrate the fundamental importance of spatial thinking for STEM learning and performance, spatial thinking remains a neglected aspect of educational practice and policy. We hope the current findings inspire new research, professional practice, and insights into the ways in which spatial thinking may be used to make learning more engaging, accessible, and equitable.”
    The research has been published by the American Psychological Association and details a meta-analysis on how spatial reasoning training impacted the mathematical abilities of 3,700 participants aged between three to 20 years old.
    In a 2021 open letter to the UK Government, the Institute of Engineering and Technology estimated a shortfall of over 173,000 workers in the science, technology, engineering and mathematics sectors, with an average of 10 unfilled roles per business in the UK. The letter, signed by 150 of the UK’s top firms, warned that if the country did not plug this skills gap, it would cost the economy £1.5bn per year. This research suggests that spatial skill training could be a novel, untapped avenue for improving STEM skills.
    Story Source:
    Materials provided by University of Surrey. Note: Content may be edited for style and length. More