More stories

  • in

    Conversations go on too long because people are too polite to end them

    By Christa Lesté-Lasserre
    People mask how they feel about an ongoing conversation and leave others unsure of whether to stop talking, suggests a study in the US
    KT images/Getty Images
    Conversations often end later than people would like – and sometimes too early – because people mask how they really feel about the ongoing dialogue, according to a study in the US. This leaves all partners in a conversation unsure of whether to stop talking.
    “People feel like it’s a social rupture to say: ‘I’m ready to go’, or to say: ‘I want to keep going although I feel like you don’t want to keep going’,” says Adam M. Mastroianni at Harvard University. “Because of that, we’re pretty skilled at not broadcasting that information.”

    Advertisement

    Mastroianni remembers attending a black-tie event and wondering how many people at the party were engaged in conversations that they really wanted to end. So, he and his colleagues later surveyed more than 800 people – 367 women and 439 men, three-quarters of whom were white – randomly recruited from a crowdsourcing marketplace website. Participants responded to questions about recent conversations they had had with a friend or family member, including how they felt about the conversation’s length and how it ended.
    The researchers also recruited more than 250 students and non-students pooled from volunteers available for studies in the Harvard University psychology department. The group, slightly under half of whom were white, included 157 women, 92 men, and three people of unspecified gender. These people participated in one-on-one conversations with another participant, who they didn’t already know, in the laboratory.

    Mastroianni’s team recorded each conversation and asked the two participants to talk about anything they liked for at least a minute. When the conversation had ended, both study participants could leave the room, where they were each – separately – quizzed about the conversation. If the conversation lasted 45 minutes, someone stepped into the room to end it.
    The conversations rarely ended when people wanted them to – whether it was one participant or both participants who wanted to stop, says Mastroianni. In fact, on average, the length of the conversations were off by about 50 per cent compared with how long people would have liked them to last.
    Mastroianni also found that that some people – 10 per cent of the participants – were actually ending the conversations even though both people wanted to continue.
    “They could have kept going; they had time left,” Mastroianni says. “But for some reason they stopped, maybe thinking they were doing a nice thing by letting the other person go.”

    Essentially, people in conversations not only want different endpoints, but they also know “precious little” about what their conversational partners really want, he says.
    That doesn’t mean the people don’t enjoy their conversations. On the contrary: when asked, his study participants said they found the conversation more entertaining than they expected it to be.
    “But what we didn’t realise as scientists – and what they didn’t realise as people – was that beneath that good time that people generally have is this whole coordination failure,” says Mastroianni.
    The study only covered people from the US and conversations might play out differently in other languages and cultures.
    Journal reference: PNAS, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2011809118
    More on these topics: More

  • in

    Neanderthal ears were tuned to hear speech just like modern humans

    By Krista Charles
    A reconstruction of a Neanderthal man and woman
    S. ENTRESSANGLE/E. DAYNES/SCIENCE PHOTO LIBRARY
    Virtual reconstructions of Neanderthal ears show that our extinct cousins had the same physical capacity for hearing as modern humans, and by inference could also make the same sounds we can – although whether they actually spoke a language is still unknown.
    “We don’t know if they had a language, but at least they had all the anatomical parts needed to have the kind of speech that we have,” says Mercedes Conde-Valverde at the University of Alcalá in Spain. “It’s not that they had the same language, not English, not Spanish, nothing like this. But if we could hear them, we would recognise that they were humans.”

    Advertisement

    Conde-Valverde and her colleagues used medical imaging software to create virtual reconstructions of Neanderthal external and middle ear cavities, based on CT scans of their skulls. With these models, they could determine the range of sounds that Neanderthals could hear, and thus probably produce as speech. This technique has previously been used to study speech and hearing in other ancient humans and chimps.

    The team also did the same for a group of fossils known as the Sima de los Huesos hominins that are thought to be the immediate ancestors of Neanderthals. The results showed that, unlike these ancestors, Neanderthals had the same capacity for hearing as modern humans.
    Neanderthal hearing was optimised towards production of consonants that often appear in modern human languages, such as “s”, “k”, “t” and “th”, in the same way our hearing is, says Conde-Valverde.
    While we don’t know if this means they had the mental capacity for language development, Conde-Valverde says that recent archaeological evidence, including stone tool use, jewellery making and art hint towards complex behaviour in Neanderthals that could indicate language ability.

    “It becomes more and more hard to dismiss the fact that probably they had some sort of speech,” says Dan Dediu at Lumière University Lyon 2 in France. It was likely very similar to ours, but not identical, he says.
    Journal reference: Nature Ecology and Evolution, DOI: 10.1038/s41559-021-01391-6
    Sign up to Our Human Story, a free monthly newsletter on the revolution in archaeology and human evolution

    More on these topics: More

  • in

    Earliest human ancestors may have swung on branches like chimps

    By Karina Shah
    The skull of Ardipithecus ramidus – a hominin that swung from branches?
    PvE/Alamy
    Our distant ancestors may have swung from branches and knuckle-walked like a chimpanzee – challenging recent thinking that the earliest hominins did neither. That is the conclusion of an analysis of 4.4-million-year-old Ardipithecus ramidus, thought to be one of the earliest known hominins.
    In popular thinking, humans are often imagined to have evolved from a chimpanzee-like ape, but many researchers now challenge this idea – particularly in light of fossil evidence from A. ramidus that was published in 2009. One well-preserved individual – nicknamed Ardi – had bones that suggested it typically walked along branches like a monkey rather than swinging below them like a chimp. This hinted that our last common ancestor with chimps also walked along branches, and that chimps evolved to swing and knuckle-walk after they branched off from hominins.

    Advertisement

    Thomas C. Prang at Texas A&M University and his colleagues disagree with this conclusion. They have taken the measurements of Ardi’s hands reported in 2009 and compared them with 416 measurements from hands across 53 species of living primates, including chimpanzees, bonobos and humans.
    “The analysis of this hand, one of the earliest hands in the human fossil record, suggests that it is chimpanzee-like, implying that both humans and chimps evolved from an ancestor that was chimp-like,” says Prang.

    They found that Ardi’s metacarpals and phalanges – the bones of the fingers and palms – were similar in size to those of living apes, with relatively large joint and knuckle dimensions. These adaptations are present in existing primates that move around forests by swinging below branches and may have helped the hominin to grasp onto branches, and even knuckle-walk.
    “Ardi also has elongated, more curved finger bones, and we see this increased elongation and curvature in animals that habitually hang from branches,” says Prang.
    Larger-bodied primates tend to hang from branches and climb trees, while smaller-bodied animals, like monkeys, are able to walk along the branches.
    “[The study] quite convincingly demonstrates that the Ardipithecus hand has some suspensory adaptations, which I think makes more sense given the body size,” says Tracy Kivell at the University of Kent, UK.

    The researchers also confirmed this using evolutionary modelling. This involved comparing traits of different primates, both living and extinct, to understand the evolutionary relationship between physical features and movement. “In short, this approach models the evolution of traits across a tree of life, which in this case includes all the species in our analysis,” says Prang.

    Understanding hand morphology of our earliest human relative brings us one step closer to explaining why humans are so different from our close relatives. This may suggest that the last common ancestor of chimpanzees and humans was relatively chimpanzee-like, before the major evolutionary shift towards bipedalism and hand dexterity.
    Tim White at the University of California, Berkeley, who discovered the A. ramidus fossil and helped describe it in 2009, remains unconvinced. “This is another failed resurrection of the antiquated notion that living chimpanzees are good models for our ancestors,” says White. He says that the Ardipithecus hand, aside from having five fingers and the ability to grasp, wasn’t specifically chimpanzee-like, as he and his colleagues originally reported in 2009.
    Sergio Almécija at the American Museum of Natural History in New York is also largely unconvinced. “We need more Miocene [epoch] ape fossils pre-dating the human-chimp split to test fundamental aspects of our last ancestor with apes,” he says.
    Journal reference: Science Advances, DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abf2474
    More on these topics: More

  • in

    Don't Miss: Kazuo Ishiguro's new novel about AIs and love

    Read
    Burn promises to change the way we think about food, exercise and life, as evolutionary biologist Herman Pontzer brings his 20 years of research experience to bear on the mysteries of human metabolism. (Buy from Amazon)

    Watch
    The Big Freeze, the Scott Polar Research Institute’s arts festival, launches online on 4 March with the European premiere of Polar Self Portraits 2, a creative project connecting artists and their work from six continents.

    Read
    Klara and the Sun is Kazuo Ishiguro’s first novel since he was awarded the Nobel prize for literature in 2017. It tells the story of an artificial friend who is learning not to invest too much in the promises of humans. (Buy from Amazon)
    More on these topics: More

  • in

    How to spend a trillion dollars to fix climate change and end poverty

    Let’s imagine you have inherited a fortune and want to solve the world’s most pressing problems. Here’s the best way to spend your money to make a difference to climate change, disease and poverty

    Life 24 February 2021
    By Rowan Hooper
    Andrea Ucini
    MOST of us have had that conversation: what would you do if you won the lottery? Pay off the mortgage, quit your job, maybe start a small business doing something you have always dreamed of. But what if you acquired a truly vast fortune – not just a few million but a trillion dollars? And what if you had to spend it on making the world a better place?
    I know, a trillion dollars – a thousand billion dollars – sounds like a vast amount of money, especially during the twin crises of recession and pandemic. But in the grand scheme of things, it isn’t. A trillion dollars is about 1 per cent of world GDP. It is what Jeff Bezos, the founder of Amazon, is on course to be worth by 2026. The world’s richest 1 per cent together own $162 trillion in assets. And it’s just one-twelfth of what governments around the world found in 2020 alone for economic stimulus packages in response to the new coronavirus.
    What could you do with such a relatively modest sum, if charged to spend it on the world’s biggest challenges? This is the central question of my book, How to Spend a Trillion Dollars, in which I choose 10 megaprojects (all things scientists are working on now) and explore what could be achieved if we showered them with money. Here we examine three of the most urgent of those challenges: solving world poverty, halting runaway climate change and curing all disease.

    Eradicate world poverty

    Perhaps the most important thing we could do for human welfare would be to alleviate poverty. According … More

  • in

    What could we do for the climate and health if money were no object?

    Bill Allsopp/Loop Images Ltd/AlaSOME readers might remember the 1985 movie Brewster’s Millions. Richard Pryor’s character has to spend $30 million in 30 days in order to inherit a $300 million fortune. This week, we update the conceit, inflating the sum to a cool $1 trillion, and set a few ground rules: the money has to be spent on projects to improve human welfare, to restore the environment and to advance science.
    It is the premise of How to Spend a Trillion Dollars, a new book by New Scientist‘s podcast editor Rowan Hooper that takes 10 megaprojects and costs them out. It is a … More

  • in

    Synchronic review: A genuinely hair-raising time travel thriller

    By Bethan Ackerley
    Signature Entertainment
    Synchronic
    Justin Benson, Aaron Moorhead
    Now available to rent online

    Advertisement

    WHEN they aren’t busy being the darlings of indie horror cinema, film-makers Justin Benson and Aaron Moorhead are, by their own admission, armchair enthusiasts of astrophysics, philosophy and futurism. That heady cocktail of interests has influenced all their films to date, but perhaps none more so than their latest and most ambitious creation: Synchronic.
    The film stars Anthony Mackie and Jamie Dornan as paramedics and friends Steve and Dennis, who are called out to a series of unusual drug overdoses across New Orleans. Although the victims are found in very different circumstances – one has been stabbed by a centuries-old sword, while others have been burned or frozen to death – they have all taken Synchronic, a designer drug based on the hallucinogen DMT.
    Aside from those grisly incidents, the first third of Synchronic is a slow-burning drama about the quiet miseries that Steve and Dennis are mired in. Steve is a disaffected womaniser who has recently been diagnosed with a brain tumour, while Dennis’s marriage is strained by a new baby and his daughter Brianna’s teenage angst. Thankfully, these personal troubles are just a vehicle for a much more intriguing concept.
    When Brianna (played by Ally Ioannides) vanishes after taking Synchronic at a frat party, Steve starts to buy up the remaining supplies. He eventually meets the drug’s creator, Dr Kermani (Ramiz Monsef), who matter-of-factly reveals that Synchronic manipulates your pineal gland, the same region of the brain as Steve’s brain tumour. It is reminiscent of the resonating device in H. P. Lovecraft’s short story From Beyond, which lets the user see alternative planes of existence. However, instead of seeing monsters from another dimension, Synchronic changes how you experience the flow of time.

    Kermani explains that time isn’t linear, instead working like a vinyl record: you play one track, but the other grooves are always there. “Synchronic is the needle,” he says, letting people travel to the past while physically remaining in the present. The catch is that you have no control over where you end up, and if you manifest in the middle of a forest fire or in the path of a rampaging bull, you will still die in the present.
    “Time works like a vinyl record: you play one track, but the other grooves are always there”
    As soon as Steve starts experimenting with Synchronic in an attempt to find Brianna, the film’s real potential emerges. He approaches the task methodically, rationing out his limited supply to establish the rules of the drug. I won’t reveal much about which time periods Steve travels to, but his encounters are surreal and upsetting in equal measure. The past is a particularly dangerous place for a Black man, and the film is at its best when it explores how time travel is disproportionately terrifying for Steve.
    While there are a few holes in the plot – why does the drug never take people to the future, for instance – the potential of Synchronic‘s central conceit is obvious. Unfortunately, while the film-makers are no strangers to small budgets, their ambitions were clearly hamstrung by a lack of funds.
    The environments in the past are severely limited, with a few brief glimpses of deserts and snowstorms being about as adventurous as the film-makers can afford. Although they make up for that with some clever tableaux and eerie, roving camerawork, you still sense that Synchronic would have benefited immeasurably from having twice as much cash, and twice as much time spent mining the horrors of history.
    All that said, Benson and Moorhead have still created a grim, uneasy thriller with truly hair-raising moments. For all that I mourn the unfulfilled potential of the concept, Synchronic is yet more evidence that these film-makers should be given the tools with which they can fully realise their mind-bending ideas.
    More on these topics: More

  • in

    The rise and fall of the mysterious culture that invented civilisation

    Proto-cities built from 6200 years ago in eastern Europe upend our ideas about when civilisation began and why people made the move from rural to urban living

    Humans 24 February 2021
    By Laura Spinney
    Stuart Mcreath
    AROUND 6200 years ago, farmers living on the eastern fringes of Europe, in what is now Ukraine, did something inexplicable. They left their neolithic villages and moved into a sparsely inhabited area of forest and steppe. There, in an area roughly the size of Belgium between the modern cities of Kiev and Odessa, they congregated at new settlements up to 20 times the size of their old ones.
    This enigmatic culture, known as the Cucuteni-Trypillia, predates the earliest known cities in Mesopotamia, a civilisation that spanned part of the Middle East, and in China. It persisted for 800 years, but then, as mysteriously as it had begun, this experiment in civilisation failed. The inhabitants left the lightest of footprints in the landscape, and no human remains have been found. “Not a pinkie, not a tooth,” says palaeogeneticist Alexey Nikitin at Grand Valley State University in Michigan.
    This puzzling lack of evidence has fuelled a lively debate about what Nikitin calls the “Dark Ages” of European prehistory. “You talk to five Trypillian archaeologists, you get five different opinions,” he says.
    But the data gap hasn’t stifled interest – quite the opposite. Several projects in recent years have tried to make sense of the Trypillian proto-cities. Despite big disagreements, what is emerging is a picture of an early and unique attempt at urbanisation. It may be the key to understanding how modern Europe emerged from the Stone Age – and even throw new light on the emergence of human civilisation in general.

    Uruk and Tell Brak, which … More